Newborns whose mothers exercise during pregnancy may become physically coordinated a little earlier than other babies, according to a captivating new study of gestation, jogging and the varying ability of tiny infants to make a fist. The study's findings add to growing evidence that physical activity during pregnancy can strengthen
Exercise during pregnancy may have lasting benefits for babies
But whether exercise during pregnancy would likewise influence a child's motor development and coordination remained unknown, May realized, and could matter. Other past research has shown that relatively poor coordination in early childhood is linked to higher risks for inactivity and obesity in adolescence and adulthood.
In essence, children who move with little ease tend to be children, and later adults, who move little.
So, for the new study, which was published this month in Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, May and her colleagues decided to look into whether exercise during pregnancy would affect babies' physical coordination after birth.
The scientists began by recruiting 71 healthy, pregnant women, most in their first trimester, and carrying a single baby. They randomly divided these volunteers into two groups, one of which continued with their normal routines, as a control.
The others began exercising, reporting to the university physiology lab three times a week, for supervised, 50-minute sessions of moderate exertion. There, they jogged, walked briskly, rode stationary bicycles or joined aerobics classes, depending on their preferences, balance and comfort as their pregnancies progressed.
The sessions continued until each woman gave birth.
All of the mothers, in both groups, delivered healthy, normal-weight infants.
A month after each birth, mother and child returned to the lab, where a paediatric physical therapist completed a standard exam of the babies' reflexes and motor skills, testing how well they controlled their heads while lying down, made a fist, rolled over, thrust out their arms, and otherwise reacted and moved.
The results were consistent. Babies whose mothers had exercised tended to perform better on almost all of the tests, suggesting that their motor skills were more advanced. These gains were especially notable among girls, who usually lag slightly behind boys at this age. But baby girls from the exercise group displayed the same relatively advanced physical capabilities as the boys in that group and more coordination than boys in the control group.
None of these variations were glaring. Every infant was healthy, with normal motor development. But the babies who had bounced along as their mothers jogged or danced were slightly ahead of the others in their ability to grip, jostle and control how they rolled.
This accelerated motor development "might encourage those children," over subsequent months and years, to be more active than youngsters whose coordination lags, May says.
The researchers did not control for home life, though, so it is possible that the mothers who exercised during pregnancy also engaged and played more with their newborns later. In that case, the improvements in motor skills would have occurred after birth and not in utero. (The researchers did consider and account for breastfeeding, which also affects physical development.)
The study also cannot tell us how maternal exercise might have goosed unborn babies' physical skills, if it did. Perhaps the infants received more blood, oxygen and nutrients through the placenta when their mothers exercised, affecting their brain and nervous system development, May says. Or the babies' bodies might have sensed maternal exertions and released growth hormones and other bio chemicals of their own that sped up the development of their motor cortexes.
May and her colleagues plan to delve into those issues in future studies.
But for now, she says, the study's results suggest that pregnant women who exercise — assuming they are healthy and have clearance from their doctor — might amplify their baby's nascent aptitude for and interest in movement.
Written by: Gretchen Reynolds
© 2019 THE NEW YORK TIMES